Latinx’s Attention to Social Context: A Comparison with European Americans and East Asians Living in the U.S.
Abstract views: 519 / PDF downloads: 420
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.29333/ejecs/1446Keywords:
attribution styles, attention to social context, culture, cultural differences, LatinxAbstract
Prior research shows that collectivistic East Asians are more sensitive to contextual information than individualistic Americans. However, limited studies have explored other collectivistic societies, such as the Latinx. Extending the current literature, we investigated attention to social context among Latinx living in the United States. Two tasks were used to test how Latinx (N=260) incorporate social context information when making attributions as compared to European Americans (N=220) and East Asians (N=144) living in the United States. In Task 1, participants made attributions about their friend’s career choice. In Task 2, participants made attributions about a character experiencing constraints in a semi-real-life situation. The findings demonstrated that when making attributions, East Asians were more attentive to social context than European Americans. Furthermore, Latinx were attentive to social context like the East Asians, but findings were not consistent across tasks. Results are discussed as a function of cultural frameworks, specifically from the perspectives of European American and East Asian cultures, and from the Latinx cultures. The role of collectivism on holistic thinking and practical implications of culture-driven attention style are discussed. Directions for future research are also suggested.
Downloads
References
Brannon, T. N., Jones Taylor, V., & Markus, H. R. (2015). “Two souls, two thoughts,” Two self-schemas: double consciousness can have positive academic consequences for African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 108, 586–609. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038992 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038992
Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y.-R. (2007). Where (Who) Are Collectives in Collectivism? Toward Conceptual Clarification of Individualism and Collectivism. Psychological Review, 114(1), 133–151. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133
Campos, B., & Kim, H. S. (2017). Incorporating the cultural diversity of family and close relationships into the study of health. American Psychologist, 72(6), 543-554. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000122 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000122
Campos, B., Ullman, J. B., Aguilera, A., & Dunkel Schetter, C. (2014). Familism and psychological health: the intervening role of closeness and social support. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 20(2), 191-201. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034094 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034094
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(9), 949-960. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298249003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298249003
D'Angiulli, A., Herdman, A., Stapells, D., & Hertzman, C. (2008). Children's event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology, 22(3), 293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.293 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.293
de Oliveira, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2017). Beyond east and west: Cognitive style in Latin America. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(10), 1554-1577. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117730816 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117730816
Fernandez, D. R., Carlson, D. S., Stepina, L. P., & Nicholson, J. D. (1997). Hofstede's country classification 25 years later. The Journal of social psychology, 137(1), 43-54. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549709595412 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549709595412
Hashimoto, T., Mojaverian, T., & Kim, H. S. (2012). Culture, interpersonal stress, and psychological distress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(4), 527-532. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022112438396 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022112438396
Hirschman, C. (1983). America’s Melting Pot Reconsidered. Annual Review of Sociology, 9, 397–423. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2946072 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.09.080183.002145
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Holloway, R. A., Waldrip, A. M., & Ickes, W. (2009). Evidence that a simpático self-schema accounts for differences in the self-concepts and social behavior of Latinos versus Whites (and Blacks). Journal of personality and social psychology, 96(5), 1012-1028. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013883 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013883
Ikizer, E. G., & Ramírez-Esparza, N. (2018). Bilinguals’ social flexibility. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 957–969. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000414 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000414
Ikizer, E. G., Ramírez-Esparza, N., & Boyd, R. (2019). #sendeanlat (You tell it too): Text analysis of tweets about sexual harassment experiences of women in Turkey. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 16(4), 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-018-0358-5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-018-0358-5
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of experimental social psychology, 3(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an Object and Its Context in Different Cultures: A Cultural Look at New Look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02432 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02432
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 1245–1267. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245
Krull, D. S., Loy, M. H.-M., Lin, J., Wang, C.-F., Chen, S., & Zhao, X. (1999). The Fundamental Fundamental Attribution Error: Correspondence Bias in Individualist and Collectivist Cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(10), 1208–1219. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299258003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299258003
Krys, K., Vignoles, V. L., de Almeida, I., & Uchida, Y. (2022). Outside the “Cultural Binary”: Understanding Why Latin American Collectivist Societies Foster Independent Selves. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(4), 1166–1187. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211029632 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211029632
Lechuga, J., Santos, B. M., Garza-Caballero, A. A., & Villarreal, R. (2011). Holistic reasoning on the other side of the world: Validation of the Analysis-Holism Scale in Mexicans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17(3), 325-330. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023881 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023881
Luegenbiehl, K., Sakamoto, K., Goto, T., Shimomura, Y., Okumura, M., Takayama, A., & Mochizuki, Y. (Producers), & Rarecho (Director). (2016). Aggretsuko [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.netflix.com.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 568-579. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205013 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205013
Markus, H.R., Kitayama, S. (1991). Cultural Variation in the Self-Concept. In: Strauss, J., Goethals, G.R. (eds) The Self: Interdisciplinary Approaches. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-8264-5_2 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-8264-5_2
Masuda, T., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Perceiver-induced constraint and attitude attribution in Japan and the US: A case for the cultural dependence of the correspondence bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(3), 409–416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.004
Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Culture and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(9), 1260-1275. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208320555 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208320555
Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural variation in correspondence bias: The critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1239–1248. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1239 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1239
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949–971. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949
Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C., Legant, P., & Marecek, J. (1973). Behavior as seen by the actor and as seen by the observer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(2), 154–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034779 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034779
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
Norenzayan, A., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture and Causal Cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(4), 132–135. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00077 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00077
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3-72. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., Rodríguez-Arauz, G., Ikizer, E. G., & Fernández-Gómez, M. J. (2019). No laughing matter: Latinas’ high quality of conversations relate to behavioral laughter. PloS one, 14(4), e0214117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214117 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214117
Ramírez-Esparza, N., Gosling, S. D., Benet-Martínez, V., Potter, J. P., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Do bilinguals have two personalities? A special case of cultural frame switching. Journal of research in personality, 40(2), 99-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.001
Reilly, E. B., Stallworthy, I. C., Mliner, S. B., Troy, M. F., Elison, J. T., & Gunnar, M. R. (2021). Infants’ abilities to respond to cues for joint attention vary by family socioeconomic status. Infancy, 26(2), 204-222. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12380 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12380
Rodríguez-Arauz, G., Ramírez-Esparza, N., Boyd, R. & Perez-Brena, N. (2017). Hablo Inglés y Español: Cultural schemas as a function of language. Frontiers in Psychology, Social Psychology 8, 885. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00885 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00885
Rodríguez-Arauz, G., Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., Ikizer, E. G., & Fernández-Gómez, M. J. (2019). You go before me, please: Behavioral politeness and interdependent self as markers of Simpatía in Latinas. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 25(3), 379-387. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000232 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000232
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 10, 173-220. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3
Senft, N., Campos, B., Shiota, M. N., & Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E. (2021). Who emphasizes positivity? An exploration of emotion values in people of Latino, Asian, and European heritage living in the United States. Emotion, 21(4), 707-719. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000737 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000737
Society for Personality and Social Psychology (SPSP). (n.d.). Research on Hispanic/latinx populations – a growing need and the current obstacles. https://spsp.org/research-hispaniclatinx-populations-growing-need-and-current-obstacles
Steidel, A. G. L., & Contreras, J. M. (2003). A new familism scale for use with Latino populations. Hispanic journal of behavioral sciences, 25(3), 312-330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986303256912 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986303256912
Tukachinsky, R. (2020). Playing a Bad Character but Endorsing a Good Cause: Actor-character Fundamental Attribution Error and Persuasion. Communication Reports, 33(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/08934215.2019.1691618 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/08934215.2019.1691618
United States Census Bureau. (2022a). Ancestry. United States Department of Commerce. https://www.census.gov/topics/population/ancestry.html
United States Census Bureau. (2022b). Hispanic Origin. United States Department of Commerce. https://www.census.gov/topics/population/hispanic-origin.html
United States Census Bureau. (2022c). Race. United States Department of Commerce. https://www.census.gov/topics/population/race.html
Varnum, M. E. W., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The Origin of Cultural Differences in Cognition: The Social Orientation Hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 9–13. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359301 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359301
Wray, A. H., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Isbell, E., Bell, T., & Neville, H. (2017). Development of selective attention in preschool-age children from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 26, 101-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.06.006 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.06.006
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2023 Journal of Ethnic and Cultural Studies
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
By submitting a manuscript to JECS, authors agree to transfer without charge the following rights to JECS upon acceptance of the manuscript: first worldwide publication rights and the right for JECS to grant permissions as JECS editors judge appropriate for the redistribution of the article, its abstract, and its metadata in professional indexing and reference services. Any revenues from such redistribution are used solely to support the continued publication and distribution of articles.
Accepted 2023-03-27
Published 2023-04-08